Body Mass Estimates and Encephalization Quotients: A Fresh Look at the Australopithecines and Homo habilis


ABSTRACTS


by Patrick H. Young

The australopithecines and Homo habilis have been publicized for years as examples of evolutionary transitional forms that launched our own human lineage. Dogmatic evolutionists have rationalized these claims on the basis of brain expansion, encephalization quotients, and bipedalism. However, any evolutionary justification for brain expansion in these extinct creatures must rest in a precise model for the determination of body mass. To insure an accurate body mass model, one must take into account whether the animal is quadruped, facultative biped, or obligatory biped. Past body mass estimates for the australopithecines and Homo habilis were based on assumptions about their bipedalism that have proven to be erroneous. When a body mass model is used accounting for the facultative bipedalism of the australopithecines and Homo habilis, the data shows that they are not highly encephalized, and hence nothing more than a microevolutionary adaptation of the pan-troglodytes.


Body Mass Estimates and Encephalization Quotients:
A Fresh Look at the Australopithecines and Homo habilis

CRSQ Vol 42 No 4 pp 217-226 March 2006



Introduction

One of the classic examples of alleged evidence for human origins that evolutionists have proposed to promulgate the evolutionary transitional status of the australopithecines and Homo habilis is a comparison of increasing cranial capacity versus their perceived evolutionary timescale (Falk, 1980; 1987; 1998; Kirkwood, 1997; Lee and Wolpoff, 2003; McHenry, 1994a). In this context, various fossilized crania are plotted against time as an attempt to demonstrate a gradual increase in brain volume from the time of our perceived most recent common ancestor to the large-brained humans we observe today (Figure 1). The goal of this type of demonstration is to provide visual evidence (however weak) that some evolutionary advancement in intelligence over time has occurred.

 

Figure 1. Extinct hominid brain mass versus perceived evolutionary date.

Realistically, the scientific validity of using cranial capacity alone as a justification for brain expansion through any perceived evolutionary timescale is suspect at best, because there is no correction for body size (McHenry and Coffing, 2000; Kappelman, 1996; Holloway, 1988; Conroy et al., 1990). It is also in significant dispute as to whether the relative brain size of the australopithecines actually did increase over perceived evolutionary time (Falk, 1987; 1998; Martin, 1990). However, the majority view in evolutionary thinking appears to indicate that the australopithecines did possess a larger relative brain size than the apes (Pilbeam and Gould, 1974). Moreover, assuming it is a valid taxon (Brace, 1979; Wood and Collard, 1999), Homo habilis is believed to be the first creature to demonstrate a measurable increase in relative brain size (Falk, 1987; Lovejoy, 1981; Hawks et al., 2000; Pilbeam and Gould, 1974; Haeusler and McHenry, 2004; Ruff et al., 1997).

While a larger brain is not necessarily a predictor of higher intelligence (Beals et al., 1984; McLeod, 1983), brain size is known to have a strong positive correlation with body size (Jerison, 1970). In other words, a larger body size usually requires more neurons and thus a correspondingly larger brain to handle the increase in total structural mass. However, neither suggests any true evolutionary adaptation (Pilbeam and Gould, 1974; Jerison, 1976), nor evolution to a higher taxonomic group (Cheek, 1981; Custance, 1968; Hummer, 1977; 1978; Cuozzo, 1977). Also, Jerison (1976) noted:

…large brains do not, in general, confer an evolutionary advantage over smaller brains. Instead large and small brains represent different but equally good evolutionary outcomes (p. 90).
 

Currently, plots using cranial capacity alone as perceived evidence for some evolutionary advancement have fallen out of favor (Duffett, 1983). Instead, evolutionists seem to have converged on the concept of encephalization which was originally proposed by Dubois (1897). The Dubois allometric factor is based on an impromptu association of brain size to body size/body function which results in a somewhat predictable logarithmic relationship (Jerison, 1970). Dubois’ initial work on encephalization has been significantly refined over the years with the extensive experimental research on primate brain size by Jerison (1970) and Martin (1990). The result of their work was the proposal of an encephalization quotient which is defined as the ratio of the true mass of a brain to the anticipated mass of the brain for a given body size (Jerison, 1970; Martin, 1990). It is essentially nothing more than an index of brain volume that can be compared to different organisms. However, the relationship fails to scale properly across different taxonomic groups of mammals (Pagel and Harvey, 1989). The allometric relationship is traditionally derived from a bivariate regression analysis of brain volume and body weight to determine the degree of encephalization of the animal in question (Sacher, 1970; Martin, 1990). Since a majority of mammals are expected to have an encephalization quotient of 1, a higher encephalization quotient implies the organism is more complex (Jerison, 1970; Martin, 1990; McHenry, 1988).

The australopithecines are an extinct group of creatures alleged to be an early evolutionary branch of divergence from chimpanzees (Ayala and Cela-Conde, 2003). Some believe they are transitional forms that are forerunners to the genus Homo (Wood and Collard, 1999; Wood, 1992; Tobias, 1991; Skelton and McHenry, 1992). Others believe they are nothing more than an extinct evolutionary branch (Pilbeam and Gould, 1974), and others believe they are simply an extinct form of ape (Cuozzo, 1977; Custance, 1968; Hummer, 1977; Lubenow, 1992; Mcleod, 1983; Oxnard, 1975; 1984). More importantly however, Pilbeam and Gould (1974) have stated that at least three principal species of Australopithecus are all just different adaptations of the same animal and their brains are all equally expanded beyond the ape grade. In this context, evolutionists have elevated the australopithecines to a pseudo-transitional status higher than the pan-troglodytes for no other reason than a larger uncorrected average brain mass.

Louis Leakey et al. (1964) were the first to propose Homo habilis as the earliest member of the genus that evolutionists also place modern humans. Homo habilis is primarily distinguished from the australopithecines by its larger brain size (Haeusler and McHenry, 2004; Pilbeam and Gould, 1974; Susman, 1994; Blumenschine et al., 2003). Homo habilis has been proposed by evolutionists as the earliest hominid to exhibit the increased brain size required to evolve human intelligence. However, the validity of placing this extinct animal in the genus Homo has been in significant dispute ever since Leakey’s discovery was first published (Wood and Collard, 1999; Kramer et al., 1995; Lieberman et al., 1988; Miller, 2000; Tobias, 2003; Hummer, 1977; Hummer, 1978; Cheek, 1981; Custance, 1968).

The primary goal of the present study is to review different body mass models and determine which is a more accurate predictor of the physical size of Australopithecus afarensis, Australopithecus africanus, Australopithecus robustus, Australopithecus boisei, and Homo habilis. Encephalization quotients are then recalculated using the accepted body mass model to determine if the evolutionary transitional status for the australopithecines and Homo habilis has any scientific validity. Additionally, this paper will evaluate the accuracy of awarding transitional status to the australopithecines over pan-troglodytes.

 

Methods of Estimating Brain Mass and Encephalization Quotient

Brain Mass

The brain mass calculation below is taken from the formula used by Ruff et al. (1997). The equation was first derived by Martin (1981) employing a least squares regression analysis to determine a bivariate relationship of brain mass and cranial capacity using the data from 27 primate species. Ruff’s regression equation (below) has a correlation coefficient of 0.995. The brain masses derived in this paper originate from the individual crania of each hominid fossil presented in Table I.

Brain mass = 1.147 x cranial capacity 0.976 (1)

Encephalization Quotient

The encephalization quotient in the form presented below is taken from Ruff et al. (1997). This equation relates brain mass and body weight, and is derived from a regression analysis of 309 extant placental mammal species with a correlation coefficient of 0.96 (Martin, 1981; Ruff et al., 1997; McHenry and Coffing, 2000).

EQ = brain mass / (11.22 x body mass0.76) (2)


Table I. Hominid Encephalization Quotient Data Using the Hominoid Body Mass Model

 

Discussion

Determination of Hominid Body Weight

While there have been several papers published attempting to relate various cranial and post-cranial fossils to hominid body weight (Aiello and Wood, 1994; Jungers, 1988; McHenry, 1988; Wolpoff, 1973; Kappelman, 1996), estimations that employ hind-limb joint diameter seems to be the best predictor of total body mass (McHenry, 1992; Jungers, 1988; Ruff and Walker, 1991; Kappelman, 1996). Several proposed regression calculations yield formulae extracted from either human data (McHenry, 1992) or from data that includes all hominoidea (Jungers, 1988; McHenry, 1992; Kappelman, 1996). The body mass formulae evaluated in this paper are based on two models proposed by McHenry (1992) utilizing human data (male and female North Americans, Khoisan, and Pygmy) and hominoid data (extant male and female apes along with the human data). Regressions using the human data assume that the animal in question is an obligatory biped, while regressions based on hominoid data include obligatory bipeds and animals that are facultative biped and quadruped.

Table II is a comparison of several hominid body weight estimates using McHenry’s human and hominoid regression formulae. Generally, body mass calculations originating from the human regression formula will bias the data towards lower body weights because obligatory biped animals possess less muscle density in their upper body. Conversely, facultative bipeds or quadrupeds possess a much higher percentage of upper body mass because their primary mode of locomotion involves the use of their upper body extremities.

While some evolutionists continue to question using human data to calculate the body weight of extinct hominids (Jungers, 1988), countless articles have appeared in secular journals presenting data on encephalization quotients focused on calculated extinct hominid body masses originating from formulae based on human models (Aiello and Dean, 2002; Ruff et al., 1997; McHenry and Coffing, 2000; McHenry, 1994a; 1994b; Wolpoff, 1973). When McHenry first proposed his human model, it was not without some skepticism. McHenry (1992) stated, “It is difficult to assess whether human or hominoid formulae give the best results” (p. 421). Jungers (1988) said that “Homo sapiens should be omitted from these models because they possess abnormally large hind-limb joints for their body size and this condition does not characterize early hominids” (p.117). It has also been demonstrated that body weight estimations for A. robustus and A. boisei based on formulae derived from hominoid post-cranial remains correlate much better with their robust jaws than estimations based on human formulae (McHenry, 1991a).

McHenry (1992) has further stated: “Common sense might favor the human equations simply because all known hominids are bipedal” (p. 421). However, generally stating that all known hominids are bipedal is extremely misleading. The australopithecines (A. afarensis, A. africanus, A. robustus / boisei) and Homo habilis are all facultative bi-pedal (Wood and Collard, 1999) which is a very different declaration than just saying they are bipedal. A facultative bipedal animal is one whose primary mode of locomotion is brachiation. Wood and Collard (1999) concluded that the Australopithecus displayed a mixed locomotor strategy of terrestrial bipedalism with an ability to climb proficiently. Conversely, the participants in the genus Homo (except for Homo habilis) are characterized by a commitment to modern humanlike terrestrial bipedalism (obligatory bipeds) and a very limited ability for brachiation. Homo habilis also had long arms along with interpretations about hand bones strongly suggesting an animal well adapted for apelike tree climbing (McHenry and Berger, 1998; Wheeler, 1992; Susman and Creel, 1979; Marzke, 1997; Mehlert, 2000).

While Wood and Collard (1999) generally concluded that the australopithecines and Homo habilis possessed a secondary locomotor strategy of terrestrial bipedalism, others have reported that this terrestrial bipedalism was primarily a postural component that aided feeding (Hunt, 1994; Lovejoy and Heiple, 1970; Straus, 1962). Spoor et al. (1994) stated:

These observations support studies of the postcranial fossil record which have concluded that H. (Homo) erectus was an obligatory biped, whereas A. africanus showed a locomotor repertoire comprising facultative bipedalism as well as arboreal climing [sic]. The labyrinthine evidence is consistent with proposals that bipedalism in australopithecines was characterized by a substantial postural component, and by the absence of more complex movements such as running and jumping (p.648, emphasis added).

Spoor et al. (1994) further admitted in the same article that their analysis of Homo habilis, while speculative, suggested that they relied less on bipedal behavior than the australopithecines.
Although Spoor, Hunt, Lovejoy, et al. believe that macroevolution is fact, they are admitting that the facultative bipedalism attributed to the australopithecines and Homo habilis is dominated by a postural component which is primarily used for feeding (similar to the extant great apes) and is completely inconsistent with the more complex human obligatory bipedal movements.

The terrestrial bipedalism exhibited by the australopithecines and Homo habilis, even though demonstrated to be nothing more than a postural component, still appears to be the predominant evolutionary criteria for their perceived transitional status and thus links them (however weakly) to humans. However, some evolutionists have argued that the locomotion strategy of the australopithecines actually differed much more from humans than from the African apes (Oxnard, 1984; Verhaegen, 1990; 1993; 1994). Some have stated that the australopithecines could have even possessed exclusive locomotor adaptations characteristic of an environment that no longer exists (Du Brul, 1977; Oxnard, 1984; Puech, 1992; Verhaegen, 1992; 1993).

Extant brachiating animals possess much greater mass in their upper body than obligatory bipeds because their upper body muscles are used more for locomotion purposes. Since the australopithecines and Homo habilis are known to be facultative bipeds, a model to calculate their body mass utilizing data only extracted from extant obligatory bipeds (humans) would result in an inaccurate number. A model recognizing the greater upper body mass of brachiating animals, with which the australopithecines are more closely associated, should produce a body weight number that is much more accurate. As an example, calculations using the McHenry hominoid model over his human model results in an average body mass percent increase of ~41% for males and ~25% for females. This data is shown in Table II.


Table II. Hominid Body Mass Comparisons Using the Human and Hominoid Formulae

 

Encephalization Quotients of the Hominids

Table I presents the measured cranial capacities, calculated brain masses and corresponding encephalization quotients for several hominid species, including Homo habilis. This paper will abide by the convention of Aiello and Dean (2002) and combine the endocranial volumes of A. robustus and A. boisei into one species for analysis purposes. Endocranial volumes for the hominids in question are taken from the Encyclopedia of Human Evolution and Prehistory (Holloway, 2000).

Some evolutionists have proposed that the fossil cranium KNM ER 1470 should be identified as Homo habilis (Blumenschine et al., 2003; Tobias, 1987; Aiello and Dean, 2002; Holloway, 1988). However, this claim continues to be in dispute (Walker and Shipman, 1996; Holloway, 2000; Lubenow, 1992; Ayala and Cela-Conde, 2003; McHenry and Coffing, 2000). So the cranial data of KNM ER 1470 will not be included as Homo habilis in this study.

Encephalization quotient calculations typically use data that are a statistical average of brain mass and body mass (Holloway, 1988, 2000; Aiello and Dean, 2002). However, the sexes of very few crania (if any) are actually known (McHenry, 1991b). This is principally because the discovery of intact crania with associated post-cranial fossils is very rare, resulting in sample sizes too limited to obtain a statistically valid average. Furthermore, White (1998) has previously stated that the coefficients of variation for endo-cranial capacities in modern great ape and human samples can range between 8% and 15%. Since there is no reason to believe that the coefficient of variation in endocranial capacity for the australopithecines and Homo habilis would be any different (Lockwood and Kimbel, 1999), and we do not know the sex of the crania in this study, encephalization quotients will be calculated using numbers that represent the widest range of calculated body masses for both male and female published in the literature. Also, since it has been demonstrated in this paper that the hominids in question are facultative bipedal, body masses originating from the hominoid model will be used to calculate the encephalization quotients and then compared to corresponding quotients originating from the human model. The final numbers should then represent the widest statistical scope of encephalization quotients conceivable from known published data.

Figure 2 is an interval plot comparing the encephalization quotients of A. afarensis and Homo habilis using human and hominoid body mass estimates, respectively. The first interval plot (Figure 2A) uses human body mass estimates and produces encephalization quotients that result in a p-value of 0.037. A p-value this low rejects the null hypothesis and says that within a 95% confidence, Homo habilis is more highly encephalized than A. afarensis. The second interval plot (Figure 2B) uses hominoid body mass estimates and generates a p-value of 0.128. A p-value of 0.128 accepts the null hypothesis and says that within a 95% confidence, Homo habilis is not more encephalized than A. afarensis. So by changing the human model to the hominoid model to accurately represent the facultative bipedalism of Homo habilis and A. afarensis, the calculated encephalization quotients of both creatures are statistically inseparable.

 

Figure 2. Interval plot comparison of encephalization quotients using the human and hominoid models.

Sexual Dimorphism

An encephalization quotient is calculated as a corrected ratio of brain mass to body mass for a particular organism. However, Table II demonstrates the well-known fact that the australopithecines and Homo habilis are sexually dimorphic (Johanson and White, 1979; McHenry, 1986; 1991a; Zihlman, 1985). Since body mass is in the denominator of the ratio in question, it is the low body weight of the dimorphic female that is the primary contributor to a higher calculated encephalization quotient. Given that the sex of very few (if any) fossilized crania of these creatures is known, it is possible that all known specimens (or a majority) could originate from only one of the sexes. Figures 3A and 3B are box plots of the australopithecines and Homo habilis encephalization quotients using male and female calculated bodyweights, respectively. Figures 3A and 3B demonstrate that the primary origin of the perceived degree of encephalization increase of Homo habilis over the australopithecines is from the calculated body weight of the sexually dimorphic female. When only male calculated bodyweights are used (Figure 3A), the encephalization quotients of the australopithecines and Homo habilis are essentially the same.

 

 

Figure 3. Male (A) and Female (B) hominid encephalization quotients using the hominoid body mass formula.

Australopithecines and Pan-Troglodytes

Evolutionists have also presented data comparing the ratio of brain mass to body mass (using the human model) versus evolutionary time as an attempt to demonstrate that extinct australopithecines are a more highly evolved version of the pan-troglodytes. Figure 4 is a scatter plot comparing the ratios achieved when using body masses originating from both the human and hominoid models. When the hominoid model is used to calculate the ratio of brain mass to body mass, the degree of brain expansion of the australopithecines and pan-troglodytes are essentially the same.

 

Figure 4. Scatter plots of the ratio human model to ratio hominoid model versus perceived evolutionary date.

Australopithecines and Homo habilis

The coefficient of variation for brain mass of Homo habilis is 9.14% (n=7). Lockwood and Kimbel (1999) have stated that there is no reason to believe that the coefficient of variation for these extinct creatures should not be as wide as 15%, just like extant humans and apes. Statistically, this means that Homo habilis could have a brain mass of 405 grams and still have a coefficient of variance less than 15%. A Homo habilis brain mass of 405 grams is well within statistical variation of brain masses calculated from australopithecine fossilized crania. Hence, when proper body mass estimates are utilized and data are corrected for known statistical variation, these extinct creatures (A. afarensis, A. africanus, A. robustus / boisei, Homo habilis), are shown to have the same degree of encephalization as extant species of apes.

Summary

All of the australopithecines including Homo habilis are facultative bipedal with brachiation as a primary mode of locomotion. Their terrestrial bipedalism is heavily dominated by a postural component. A brachiating animal must have more mass in the upper body than obligatory bipeds (humans) because of their primary locomotion repertoire. A model for calculating body mass that only includes obligatory bipeds would be skewed to an abnormally small value because of the obvious upper body mass differences of a facultative biped compared to an obligatory biped. Hence a model that includes data from animals that practice brachiation, etc. would more accurately predict the true body mass of the australopithecines and Homo habilis.

When a body mass model based on McHenry’s (1992) hominoid data is used in encephalization quotient calculations, the brain advancement of Homo habilis and the australopithecines is statistically the same. Evolutionists have embraced Homo habilis as the first step upward in brain mass that eventually led to humans. Within statistical error, when body mass calculations are corrected for (1) a primary locomotor strategy of brachiation, (2) a terrestrial bipedalism that is postural, and (3) known sexual dimorphism, it is no more encephalized than the australopithecines and pan-troglodytes.

Australopithecines are generally labeled as “hominids” because there are features they lack that are epitomized in the living apes and they exhibit what is interpreted as humanlike attributes. However, it is frequently argued that their perceived humanlike characteristics might just be primitive because many of these characteristics are found in premature African apes. They certainly are not more encephalized than extant chimpanzees. Therefore it is more probable that the australopithecines are phylogenetically connected to African apes and should not be considered an evolutionary transitional link toward becoming humans (Kleindienst, 1975; Goodman, 1982; Oxnard, 1975; Hasegawa et al., 1985; Edelstein, 1987; Verhaegen, 1990; 1994).


References

CRSQ: Creation Research Society Quarterly

Am. J. Phys. Anthro.: American Journal of Physical Anthropology

J. Hum. Evol.: Journal of Human Evolution

Hum. Evol.: Human Evolution

Aiello, L.C. and B.A. Wood. 1994. Cranial variables as predictors of hominine body mass. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 95:409–426.

Aiello, L.C., and C. Dean. 2002. An Introduction to Human Evolutionary Anatomy. Academic Press, Boston, MA.

Ayala, F.J., and C.J. Cela-Conde. 2003. Genera of the human lineage. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100(13):7684–7689.

Beals, K.L., C.L. Smith, and S.M. Dodd. 1984. Brain Size, cranial morphology, climate, and time machines. Current Anthropology 25(3):301–330.

Blumenschine, R.J., C.R. Peters, F.T. Masao, R.J. Clark, A.L. Deino, R.L. Hay, C.C. Swisher, I.G. Stanistreet, G.M. Ashley, L.J. McHenry, N.E. Sikes, N.J. van der Merwe, J.C. Tactilkos, A.E.

Cushing, D.M. Deocampo, J.D. Njau, and J.I. Ebert. 2003. Late Pliocene Homo and hominid land use from western Olduvai Gorge, Tanzania. Science 299:1217–1221.

Brace, C.L. 1979. Biological parameters and Pleistocene hominid lifeways. In Bernstein, I.S. and E.O. Smith (editors), Primate Ecology and Human Origins: Ecological Influences on Social Organization, pp. 263–289. Garland Press, New York, NY.

Cheek, D.W. 1981. The creationist and Neo-Darwinian view concerning the origin of the order primates compared and contrasted: a preliminary analysis. CRSQ 18(2):93–110.

Cuozzo, J.W. 1977. Skull 1470 – a new look. CRSQ 14(3):173–176.

Conroy, G.C., M.W. Vannier, and P.V. Tobias. 1990. Endocranial features of Australopithecus africanus revealed by 2- and 3-D computed tomography. Science 247:838–840.

Custance, A.C. 1968. Fossil man in the light of the record in Genesis. CRSQ 5(1):5–22.

Du Brul, E.L. 1977. Early hominid feeding mechanisms. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 47: 305–320.

Dubois, E. 1897. Sur le rapport du poids de l’encephale avec la grandeur du corps chez les mammiferes. Bulletin of the Society of Anthropology Paris 8:337–376.

Duffett, G. 1983. Some implications of variant cranial capacities for the best-preserved Australopithecine skull specimens. CRSQ 20(2):96–104.

Edelstein, S.J. 1987. An alternative paradigm for hominoid evolution. Hum. Evol. 2:169–174.
Falk, D. 1980. Hominid brain evolution: the approach from paleoneurology. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology 23:93 107.

–—–—–. 1987. Hominid paleoneurology. Annual Review of Anthropology 16:13–30.

–—–—–. 1998. Hominid brain evolution: looks can be deceiving. Science 280:1714–1717.

Goodman, M. 1982. Biomolecular evidence on human origins from the standpoint of Darwinian theory. Human Biology 54:247–264.

Haeusler, M., and H.M. McHenry. 2004. Body proportions of Homo habilis reviewed. J. Hum. Evol. 46:433–465.

Hasegawa, M., H. Kishino, and T. Yano. 1985. Dating of the human-ape splitting by a molecular clock of mitochondrial DNA. Journal of Molecular Evolution 2:160–174.

Hawks, J., K. Hunley, S.H. Lee, and M. Wolpoff. 2000. Population bottlenecks and Pleistocene human evolution. Molecular Biology and Evolution 17(1):2–22.

Holloway, R.L. 1988. “Robust” Australopithecine brain endo-casts: some preliminary observations. In Grine, F.E. (editor), Evolutionary History of the “Robust” Australopithecines, pp. 97–105. Aldine de Gruyter, New York, NY.

Holloway, R.L. 2000. Brain. In Delson, E., and I Tattersall (editors), Encyclopedia of Human Evolution and Prehistory, pp. 141–149. Garland Publishing, New York, NY.

Hummer, C.C. 1977. A plea for caution about Skull 1470. CRSQ 14(3):168–172.

–—–—–. 1978. Unthinking Homo habilis. CRSQ 15(4):212–214.

Hunt, K.D. 1994. The evolution of human bipedality: ecology and functional morphology. J. Hum. Evol. 26(3):183–202.

Jerison, H.J. 1970. Gross Brain Indices and the Analysis of Fossil Endocasts. In Noback, C.R. and W. Montagna (editors), The Primate Brain, pp. 225–243. Appleton-Century-Crofts Educational Division/Meredith Corporation, New York, NY.

–—–—–. 1976. Paleoneurology and the evolution of mind. Scientific American 234:90–101.

Johanson, D.C. and T.D. White. 1979. A systematic assessment of early African hominids. Science 203:321–330.

Jungers, W.L. 1988. New estimates of body size in Australopithecines. In Grine, F.E. (editor), Evolutionary History of the “Robust” Australopithecines, pp. 115–125. Aldine de Gruyter, New York, NY.

Kappelman, J. 1996. The evolution of body mass and relative brain size in fossil Hominids. J. Hum. Evol. 30:243–276.

Kirkwood, T.B.L. 1997. The origins of human ageing. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences 352(1363):1765–1772.

Kleindienst, M.R. 1975. On new perspectives on ape and human evolution. Current Anthropology 16:644– 646.

Kramer, A., S.M. Donnelly, J.H. Kidder, S.D. Ousley, and S.M. Olah. 1995. Craniometric variation in large-bodied hominids: testing the single-species hypothesis for Homo habilis. J. Hum. Evol. 29:443–462.

Leakey, L.S.B., P.B. Tobias, and J.R. Napier. 1964. A new species of the genus Homo from the Oldvuai Gorge. Nature 2002:7–9.

Lee S.H., and M.H. Wolpoff. 2003. The pattern of evolution in Pleistocene human brain size. Paleobiology 29(2):186–196.

Lieberman, D.E., B.A. Wood, and D.R. Pilbeam. 1988. A probabilistic approach to the problem of sexual dimorphism in Homo habilis: a comparison of KNM-ER 1470 and KMN-ER 1813. J. Hum. Evol. 17:503–511.

Lockwood, G.A., and W.H. Kimbel. 1999. Endocranial Capacity of Early Hominids. Science 283:9.

Lovejoy, C.O., and K.G. Heiple. 1970. A reconstruction of the femur of Australopithecus africanus. J. Phys. Anthro. 32:33–40.

–—–—–. 1981. The origin of man. Science 211:341–350.

Lubenow, M.L. 1992. Bones of Contention: A Creationists Assessment of Human Fossils. Baker Books, Grand Rapids, MI.

Martin, R.D. 1981. Relative brain size and basal metabolic rate in terrestrial vertebrates. Nature 293:57–60.

–—–—–. 1990. Primate Origins and Evolution: a Phylogenetic Reconstruction. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ.

Marzke, M.W. 1997. Precision grips, hand morphology, and tools. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 102:91–110.

McHenry H.M. 1986. Size variation in the postcranium of Australopithecus afarensis and extant species of Hominoidea. J. Hum. Evol. 15:149–156.

–—–—–. 1988. New estimates of body weight in early hominids and their significance to encephalization and megadontia in “Robust” Australopithecines. In Grine, F.E. (editor), Evolutionary history of the “Robust” Australopithecines, pp. 133–148. Aldine de Gruyter, New York, NY.

–—–—–. 1991a. Petite bodies of the “Robust” Australopithecines. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 86:445–454.

–—–—–. 1991b. Femoral lengths and stature in Plio-Pleistocene hominids. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 85:149–158.

–—–—–. 1992. Body size and proportions in early hominids. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 87:407–431.

–—–—–. 1994a. Behavioral ecological implications of early hominid body size. J. Hum. Evol. 27:77–87.

–—–—–. 1994b. Tempo and mode in human evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 91:6780–6786.

McHenry, H.M., and L.R. Berger. 1998. Body proportions in Australopithecus afarensis and A. africanus and the origin of the genus Homo. J. Hum. Evol. 35:1–22.

McHenry, H.M., and K. Coffing. 2000. Australopithecus to Homo: transformations in body and mind. Annual Review of Anthropology 29:125–146.

McLeod, K.C. 1983. Studying the human brain. CRSQ 20(2):75–79.

Mehlert, A.W. 2000. Australopithecines – the extinct southern apes of Africa: a fresh light on the status? TJ 14(3):91–99.

Miller, J.M. 2000. Craniofacial variation in Homo habilis: an analysis of the evidence for multiple species. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 112:103–128.

Oxnard, C.E. 1975. Uniqueness and Diversity in Human Evolution: Morphometric Studies of the Australopithecines. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, Ill.

–—–—–. 1984. The Order of Man. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT.

Pagel, M.D. and P.H. Harvey. 1989. Taxonomic Differences in the Scaling of Brain on body weight among mammals. Science 244:1589–1593.

Pilbeam, D. and S.J. Gould. 1974. Size and scaling in human evolution. Science 186:892–901.
Puech, P.F. 1992. Microwear studies of early African hominid teeth. Scanning Microscopy 6:1083–1088.

Ruff, C.B. and A. Walker. 1991. Body size of KNM-WT 15000. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 12:155–159.

Ruff, C.B., E. Trinkaus, and T.W. Holliday, 1997. Body mass and encephalization in Pleistocene Homo. Nature 387:173–176.

Sacher, G.A. 1970. Allometric and factorial analysis of brain structure in insectivores. In Noback, C.R. and W. Montagna (editors), The Primate Brain, pp. 245–287. Appleton-Century-Crofts Educational Division/Meredith Corporation, New York, NY.

Skelton, R.R., and H.M. McHenry. 1992. Evolutionary relationships among early hominids. J. Hum. Evol. 23:309–349.

Spoor, F., B.A. Wood, and F. Zonneveld. 1994. Implications of Early hominid labyrinthine morphology for evolution of human bipedal locomotion. Nature 369:645–648.

Straus, W.L. 1962. Fossil evidence of the evolution of the erect bipedal posture. Clinical Orthopaedics and Related Research 25:9–19.

Susman, R.L., and N. Creel. 1979. Functional and morphological affinities of the subadult hand (O.H. 7) from Olduvai Gorge. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 51:311–332.

–—–—–. 1994. Fossil evidence for early hominid tool use. Science. 265:1570–1573.

Tobias, P.V. 1987. The brain of Homo habilis: a new level of organization in cerebral evolution. J. Hum. Evol. 53:173–187.

–—–—–. 1991. Olduvai Gorge. Vol. 4: The Skulls, Endocasts and Teeth of Homo habilis. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

–—–—–. 2003. Encore Olduvai. Science 299:1193–1194.

Verhaegen, M. 1990. African ape ancestry. Hum. Evol. 5: 295–297.

–—–—–. 1992. Did robust australopithecines partly feed on hard parts of Gramineae? Hum. Evol. 7:63–64.

–—–—–. 1993. Aquatic versus savanna: comparative and paleo-environmental evidence. Nutrition and Health 9:165–191.

–—–—–. 1994. Australopithecines: ancestors of the African apes? Hum. Evol. 9:121–139.
Walker, A., and P. Shipman. 1996. The Wisdom of the Bones: in Search of Human Origins. Vintage Books, New York, NY.

Wheeler, P.E. 1992. The influence of stature and body form on hominid energy and water budgets; a comparison of Australopithecus and early Homo physiques. J. Hum. Evol. 24:13–28.

White, T.D. 1998. “No surprises?” With response by Dean Falk. Science 281:45–48.

Wolpoff, M.H. 1973. Posterior tooth size, body size, and diet in South African Gracile Australopithecines. Am. J. Phys. Anthro. 39:375–393.

Wood, B.A. 1992. Origin and evolution of the genus Homo. Nature 355:783–790.

Wood, B.A., and M. Collard. 1999. The Human Genus. Science 284:65–71.

Zihlman, A.L. 1985. Australopithecus afarensis: two sexes or two species? In Tobias, P.V. (editor). Hominid Evolution: Past Present and Future, pp. 213–220. Alan R. Liss, New York, NY.